Pubmed    Pubmed Central
uBio Home | uBioRSS

WebSearchLiteratureMolecularImages

 uBio  Web Results 1 - 10 of about 952

Synonyms:
   Taeniopygia guttata (Zebra Finch) 

Broader Terms:
   Passeriformes (perching birds) 
   Taeniopygia 

More Specific:
   Taeniopygia guttata castanotis (Chestnut-eared Finch) 
   Taeniopygia guttata guttata (Zebra Finch) 
 
 
Latest Articles on Taeniopygia guttata Viellot 1817 from uBioRSS
Developing birdsong learning preferences - Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sci...
Developmental stress and reproduction - Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sci...


Taeniopygia guttata
Petr Pivo˛ka - BioLib

External Resources:

Common Names: Zebra Finch, Diamant mandarin



1.  Serotonin expression in the song circuitry of adult male zebra finches.LinkIT
Yip PK, Schmitzberger M, Al-Hasan M, George J, Tripoliti E, Michael-Titus AT, Clayton D, Priestley JV
Neuroscience, 2020
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=pubmed&dopt=Abstract&list_uids=0

2.  Physiological responses of wild zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) to heatwaves.LinkIT
Cooper CE, Hurley LL, Deviche P, Griffith SC
The Journal of experimental biology, 2020
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=pubmed&dopt=Abstract&list_uids=0

3.  Network dynamics underlie learning and performance of birdsong.LinkIT
Bertram R, Hyson RL, Brunick AJ, Flores D, Johnson F
Current opinion in neurobiology, 2020
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=pubmed&dopt=Abstract&list_uids=0

4.  Offspring performance is well buffered against stress experienced by ancestors.LinkIT
Pei Y, Forstmeier W, Kempenaers B
Evolution; international journal of organic evolution Evolution Offspring performance is well buffered against stress experienced by ancestors. 10.1111/evo.14026 Evolution should render individuals resistant to stress and particularly to stress experienced by ancestors. However, many studies report negative effects of stress experienced by one generation on the performance of subsequent generations. To assess the strength of such transgenerational effects we propose a strategy aimed at overcoming the problem of type I errors when testing multiple proxies of stress in multiple ancestors against multiple offspring performance traits, and we apply it to a large observational dataset on captive zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata). We combine clear one-tailed hypotheses with steps of validation, meta-analytic summary of mean effect sizes, and independent confirmatory testing. We find that drastic differences in early growth conditions (nestling body mass 8 days after hatching varied sevenfold between 1.7 and 12.4 g) had only moderate direct effects on adult morphology (95% confidence interval [CI]: r = 0.19-0.27) and small direct effects on adult fitness traits (r = 0.02-0.12). In contrast, we found no indirect effects of parental or grandparental condition (r = -0.017 to 0.002; meta-analytic summary of 138 effect sizes), and mixed evidence for small benefits of matching environments between parents and offspring, as the latter was not robust to confirmatory testing in independent datasets. This study shows that evolution has led to a remarkable robustness of zebra finches against undernourishment. Our study suggests that transgenerational effects are absent in this species, because CIs exclude all biologically relevant effect sizes. ┬ę 2020 The Authors. Evolution published by Wiley Periodicals LLC on behalf of The Society for the Study of Evolution. Pei Yifan Y https://orcid.org/0000-0002-2411-4454 Department of Behavioural Ecology and Evolutionary Genetics, Max Planck Institute for Ornithology, Seewiesen, 82319, Germany. Forstmeier Wolfgang W https://orcid.org/0000-0002-5984-8925 Department of Behavioural Ecology and Evolutionary Genetics, Max Planck Institute for Ornithology, Seewiesen, 82319, Germany. Kempenaers Bart B https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7505-5458 Department of Behavioural Ecology and Evolutionary Genetics, Max Planck Institute for Ornithology, Seewiesen, 82319, Germany. eng Max-Planck-Gesellschaft Journal Article 2020 05 28 United States Evolution 0373224 0014-3820 IM Anticipatory effect condition transfer early developmental stress life span morphology multiple testing quantitative genetics reproductive performance resilience transgenerational effect 2020 03 30 2020 05 14 2020 05 17 2020 5 29 6 0 2020 5 29 6 0 2020 5 29 6 0 aheadofprint 32463119 10.1111/evo.14026 LITERATURE CITED, 2020</i></font><br><font color=#008000>http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=pubmed&dopt=Abstract&list_uids=0<br></font></span><br>5.  <a href=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=pubmed&dopt=Abstract&list_uids=0 class=title>Effects of sublethal application of Deepwater Horizon oil to bird eggs on embryonic heart and metabolic rate.</a><a href=http://ubio.org/tools/linkit.php?map%5B%5D=all&link_type=2&url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=pubmed&dopt=Abstract&list_uids=0><img src=linkit.png border=0 title='LinkIT' alt='LinkIT'></a> <br><span class=j>Goodchild CG, Grisham K, Belden J, DuRant SE<br><font color=gray><i>Conservation biology : the journal of the Society for Conservation Biology, 2020</i></font><br><font color=#008000>http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=pubmed&dopt=Abstract&list_uids=0<br></font></span><br>6.  <a href=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=pubmed&dopt=Abstract&list_uids=0 class=title>Individual vocal recognition in zebra finches relies on song syllable structure rather than song syllable order.</a><a href=http://ubio.org/tools/linkit.php?map%5B%5D=all&link_type=2&url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=pubmed&dopt=Abstract&list_uids=0><img src=linkit.png border=0 title='LinkIT' alt='LinkIT'></a> <br><span class=j>Geberzahn N, Der├ęgnaucourt S<br><font color=gray><i>The Journal of experimental biology, 2020</i></font><br><font color=#008000>http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=pubmed&dopt=Abstract&list_uids=0<br></font></span><br>7.  <a href=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=pubmed&dopt=Abstract&list_uids=0 class=title>Familiarity-The bridge from social interactions to relationships?</a><a href=http://ubio.org/tools/linkit.php?map%5B%5D=all&link_type=2&url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=pubmed&dopt=Abstract&list_uids=0><img src=linkit.png border=0 title='LinkIT' alt='LinkIT'></a> <br><span class=j>Freeberg TM<br><font color=gray><i>Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983), 2020</i></font><br><font color=#008000>http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=pubmed&dopt=Abstract&list_uids=0<br></font></span><br>8.  <a href=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=pubmed&dopt=Abstract&list_uids=0 class=title>The stressed brain: regional and stress-related corticosterone and stress-regulated gene expression in the adult zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata).</a><a href=http://ubio.org/tools/linkit.php?map%5B%5D=all&link_type=2&url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=pubmed&dopt=Abstract&list_uids=0><img src=linkit.png border=0 title='LinkIT' alt='LinkIT'></a> <br><span class=j>Rensel MA, Schlinger BA<br><font color=gray><i>Journal of neuroendocrinology, 2020</i></font><br><font color=#008000>http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=pubmed&dopt=Abstract&list_uids=0<br></font></span><br>9.  <a href=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=pubmed&dopt=Abstract&list_uids=0 class=title>Manipulations of sensory experiences during development reveal mechanisms underlying vocal learning biases in zebra finches.</a><a href=http://ubio.org/tools/linkit.php?map%5B%5D=all&link_type=2&url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=pubmed&dopt=Abstract&list_uids=0><img src=linkit.png border=0 title='LinkIT' alt='LinkIT'></a> <br><span class=j>James LS, Davies R, Mori C, Wada K, Sakata JT<br><font color=gray><i>Developmental neurobiology Dev Neurobiol Manipulations of sensory experiences during development reveal mechanisms underlying vocal learning biases in zebra finches. 10.1002/dneu.22754 Biological predispositions in learning can bias and constrain the cultural evolution of social and communicative behaviors (e.g., speech and birdsong), and lead to the emergence of behavioral and cultural "universals." For example, surveys of laboratory and wild populations of zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) document consistent patterning of vocal elements ("syllables") with respect to their acoustic properties (e.g., duration, mean frequency). Furthermore, such universal patterns are also produced by birds that are experimentally tutored with songs containing randomly sequenced syllables ("tutored birds"). Despite extensive demonstrations of learning biases, much remains to be uncovered about the nature of biological predispositions that bias song learning and production in songbirds. Here, we examined the degree to which "innate" auditory templates and/or biases in vocal motor production contribute to vocal learning biases and production in zebra finches. Such contributions can be revealed by examining acoustic patterns in the songs of birds raised without sensory exposure to song ("untutored birds") or of birds that are unable to hear from early in development ("early-deafened birds"). We observed that untutored zebra finches and early-deafened zebra finches produce songs with positional variation in some acoustic features (e.g., mean frequency) that resemble universal patterns observed in tutored birds. Similar to tutored birds, early-deafened birds also produced song motifs with alternation in acoustic features across adjacent syllables. That universal acoustic patterns are observed in the songs of both untutored and early-deafened birds highlights the contribution motor production biases to the emergence of universals in culturally transmitted behaviors. ┬ę 2020 Wiley Periodicals, LLC. James Logan S LS Department of Biology, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada. Centre for Research in Brain, Language and Music, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada. Davies Ronald R Jr Department of Biology, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada. Mori Chihiro C Graduate School of Life Science, Hokkaido University, Sapporo, Japan. Wada Kazuhiro K Graduate School of Life Science, Hokkaido University, Sapporo, Japan. Faculty of Science, Hokkaido University, Sapporo, Japan. Sakata Jon T JT https://orcid.org/0000-0003-2285-9310 Department of Biology, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada. Centre for Research in Brain, Language and Music, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada. Center for Studies of Behavioral Neurobiology, Concordia University, Montreal, QC, Canada. eng Heller Award Centre for Research on Brain, Language and Music 2018-PR-206494 Fonds de Recherche Nature et Technologies 05016 Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada JP17H06380 Japan Society for the Promotion of Science JP19H04888 Japan Society for the Promotion of Science JP18H02520 Japan Society for the Promotion of Science Journal Article 2020 04 24 United States Dev Neurobiol 101300215 1932-8451 IM behavioral universals biological predispositions birdsong cultural evolution songbird 2020 02 06 2020 04 10 2020 04 20 2020 4 25 6 0 2020 4 25 6 0 2020 4 25 6 0 aheadofprint 32330360 10.1002/dneu.22754 REFERENCES, 2020</i></font><br><font color=#008000>http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=pubmed&dopt=Abstract&list_uids=0<br></font></span><br>10.  <a href=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=pubmed&dopt=Abstract&list_uids=0 class=title>Different Reactions of Zebra Finches and Bengalese Finches to a Three-Component Mixture of Anesthetics.</a><a href=http://ubio.org/tools/linkit.php?map%5B%5D=all&link_type=2&url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=pubmed&dopt=Abstract&list_uids=0><img src=linkit.png border=0 title='LinkIT' alt='LinkIT'></a> <br><span class=j>Ikebuchi M, Okanoya K, Bischof HJ<br><font color=gray><i>Zoological science, 2020</i></font><br><font color=#008000>http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=pubmed&dopt=Abstract&list_uids=0<br></font></span><br><br><br><table cellspacing=0 cellpadding=0 align=center><tr valign=bottom><td align=center><img src=p.png border=0></td><td align=center><img src=o_red.png border=0></td><td align=center><a href=http://ubio.org/portal/index.php?search=Taeniopygia+guttata+Viellot+1817&category=l&client=pubmed&startPage=2><img src=o_yellow.png border=0></a></td><td align=center><a href=http://ubio.org/portal/index.php?search=Taeniopygia+guttata+Viellot+1817&category=l&client=pubmed&startPage=3><img src=o_yellow.png border=0></a></td><td align=center><a href=http://ubio.org/portal/index.php?search=Taeniopygia+guttata+Viellot+1817&category=l&client=pubmed&startPage=4><img src=o_yellow.png border=0></a></td><td align=center><a href=http://ubio.org/portal/index.php?search=Taeniopygia+guttata+Viellot+1817&category=l&client=pubmed&startPage=5><img src=o_yellow.png border=0></a></td><td align=center><a href=http://ubio.org/portal/index.php?search=Taeniopygia+guttata+Viellot+1817&category=l&client=pubmed&startPage=6><img src=o_yellow.png border=0></a></td><td align=center><a href=http://ubio.org/portal/index.php?search=Taeniopygia+guttata+Viellot+1817&category=l&client=pubmed&startPage=7><img src=o_yellow.png border=0></a></td><td align=center><a href=http://ubio.org/portal/index.php?search=Taeniopygia+guttata+Viellot+1817&category=l&client=pubmed&startPage=8><img src=o_yellow.png border=0></a></td><td align=center><a href=http://ubio.org/portal/index.php?search=Taeniopygia+guttata+Viellot+1817&category=l&client=pubmed&startPage=9><img src=o_yellow.png border=0></a></td><td align=center><a href=http://ubio.org/portal/index.php?search=Taeniopygia+guttata+Viellot+1817&category=l&client=pubmed&startPage=10><img src=o_yellow.png border=0></a></td><td align=center><a href=http://ubio.org/portal/index.php?search=Taeniopygia+guttata+Viellot+1817&category=l&client=pubmed&startPage=2><img src=rtal.png border=0></a></td></tr><td align=center></td><td align=center>1</td><td align=center><a href=http://ubio.org/portal/index.php?search=Taeniopygia+guttata+Viellot+1817&category=l&client=pubmed&startPage=2>2</a></td><td align=center><a href=http://ubio.org/portal/index.php?search=Taeniopygia+guttata+Viellot+1817&category=l&client=pubmed&startPage=3>3</a></td><td align=center><a href=http://ubio.org/portal/index.php?search=Taeniopygia+guttata+Viellot+1817&category=l&client=pubmed&startPage=4>4</a></td><td align=center><a href=http://ubio.org/portal/index.php?search=Taeniopygia+guttata+Viellot+1817&category=l&client=pubmed&startPage=5>5</a></td><td align=center><a href=http://ubio.org/portal/index.php?search=Taeniopygia+guttata+Viellot+1817&category=l&client=pubmed&startPage=6>6</a></td><td align=center><a href=http://ubio.org/portal/index.php?search=Taeniopygia+guttata+Viellot+1817&category=l&client=pubmed&startPage=7>7</a></td><td align=center><a href=http://ubio.org/portal/index.php?search=Taeniopygia+guttata+Viellot+1817&category=l&client=pubmed&startPage=8>8</a></td><td align=center><a href=http://ubio.org/portal/index.php?search=Taeniopygia+guttata+Viellot+1817&category=l&client=pubmed&startPage=9>9</a></td><td align=center><a href=http://ubio.org/portal/index.php?search=Taeniopygia+guttata+Viellot+1817&category=l&client=pubmed&startPage=10>10</a></td><td align=center><a href=http://ubio.org/portal/index.php?search=Taeniopygia+guttata+Viellot+1817&category=l&client=pubmed&startPage=2>┬╗</a></td></tr></table></table></tr></table></td><script src="http://www.google-analytics.com/urchin.js" type="text/javascript"> </script> <script type="text/javascript"> _uacct = "UA-634822-1"; urchinTracker(); </script> </BODY> </HTML>